Skip to content

Latest commit

 

History

History
831 lines (420 loc) · 35.9 KB

biases.md

File metadata and controls

831 lines (420 loc) · 35.9 KB

Belief, decision-making and behavioral

False priors

Agent detection

The inclination to presume the purposeful intervention of a sentient or intelligent agent.

Automation bias

The tendency to depend excessively on automated systems which can lead to erroneous automated information overriding correct decisions.

Gender bias

A widely held set of implicit biases that discriminate against a gender. For example, the assumption that women are less suited to jobs requiring high intellectual ability. Or the assumption that people or animals are male in the absence of any indicators of gender. Or the assumption that academia discriminates against women even as they outnumber men in college and graduate school in the US, and earn the majority of undergraduate and graduate degrees.

Stereotyping

Expecting a member of a group to have certain characteristics without having actual information about that individual.

Prospect theory

Ambiguity effect

The tendency to avoid options for which the probability of a favorable outcome is unknown.

Disposition effect

The tendency to sell an asset that has accumulated in value and resist selling an asset that has declined in value.

Dread aversion

Just as losses yield double the emotional impact of gains, dread yields double the emotional impact of savouring.

Endowment effect

The tendency for people to demand much more to give up an object than they would be willing to pay to acquire it.

Loss aversion

The perceived disutility of giving up an object is greater than the utility associated with acquiring it. (see also Sunk cost effects and endowment effect).

Pseudocertainty effect

The tendency to make risk-averse choices if the expected outcome is positive, but make risk-seeking choices to avoid negative outcomes.

Status quo bias

The tendency to like things to stay relatively the same (see also loss aversion, endowment effect, and system justification).

System justification

The tendency to defend and bolster the status quo. Existing social, economic, and political arrangements tend to be preferred, and alternatives disparaged, sometimes even at the expense of individual and collective self-interest. (See also status quo bias.)

Anchoring bias

Anchoring or focalism

The tendency to rely too heavily, or "anchor", on one trait or piece of information when making decisions (usually the first piece of information acquired on that subject).

Conservatism bias(belief revision)

The tendency to revise one's belief insufficiently when presented with new evidence.

Functional fixedness

Limits a person to using an object only in the way it is traditionally used.

Law of the instrument

An over-reliance on a familiar tool or methods, ignoring or under-valuing alternative approaches. "If all you have is a hammer, everything looks like a nail."

Availability bias

Anthropocentric thinking

The tendency to use human analogies as a basis for reasoning about other, less familiar, biological phenomena.

Anthropomorphism orpersonification

The tendency to characterize animals, objects, and abstract concepts as possessing human-like traits, emotions, and intentions. The opposite bias, of not attributing feelings or thoughts to another person, is dehumanised perception, a type of objectification.

Attentional bias

The tendency of perception to be affected by recurring thoughts.

Availability heuristic

The tendency to overestimate the likelihood of events with greater "availability" in memory, which can be influenced by how recent the memories are or how unusual or emotionally charged they may be.

Frequency illusion orBaader–Meinhof phenomenon

The frequency illusion is that once something has been noticed then every instance of that thing is noticed, leading to the belief it has a high frequency of occurrence (a form of selection bias). The Baader–Meinhof phenomenon is the illusion where something that has recently come to one's attention suddenly seems to appear with improbable frequency shortly afterwards. It was named after an incidence of frequency illusion in which the Baader–Meinhof Group was mentioned.

Implicit association

The speed with which people can match words depends on how closely they are associated.

Salience bias

The tendency to focus on items that are more prominent or emotionally striking and ignore those that are unremarkable, even though this difference is often irrelevant by objective standards.

Selection bias

The tendency to notice something more when something causes us to be more aware of it, such as when we buy a car, we tend to notice similar cars more often than we did before. They are not suddenly more common – we just are noticing them more. Also called the Observational Selection Bias.

Survivorship bias

Concentrating on the people or things that "survived" some process and inadvertently overlooking those that didn't because of their lack of visibility.

Well travelled road effect

Underestimation of the duration taken to traverse oft-travelled routes and overestimation of the duration taken to traverse less familiar routes.

Uncategorized

Attribute substitution

Occurs when a judgment has to be made (of a target attribute) that is computationally complex, and instead a more easily calculated heuristic attribute is substituted. This substitution is thought of as taking place in the automatic intuitive judgment system, rather than the more self-aware reflective system.

Curse of knowledge

When better-informed people find it extremely difficult to think about problems from the perspective of lesser-informed people.

Declinism

The predisposition to view the past favorably (rosy retrospection) and future negatively.

Dunning–Kruger effect

The tendency for unskilled individuals to overestimate their own ability and the tendency for experts to underestimate their own ability.

Empathy gap

The tendency to underestimate the influence or strength of feelings, in either oneself or others.

End-of-history illusion

The age-independent belief that one will change less in the future than one has in the past.

Exaggerated expectation

The tendency to expect or predict more extreme outcomes than those outcomes that actually happen.

Form function attribution bias

In human–robot interaction, the tendency of people to make systematic errors when interacting with a robot. People may base their expectations and perceptions of a robot on its appearance (form) and attribute functions which do not necessarily mirror the true functions of the robot.

Hard–easy effect

The tendency to overestimate one's ability to accomplish hard tasks, and underestimate one's ability to accomplish easy tasks

Hindsight bias

Sometimes called the "I-knew-it-all-along" effect, the tendency to see past events as being predictable at the time those events happened.

IKEA effect

The tendency for people to place a disproportionately high value on objects that they partially assembled themselves, such as furniture from IKEA, regardless of the quality of the end product.

Impact bias

The tendency to overestimate the length or the intensity of the impact of future feeling states.

Information bias

The tendency to seek information even when it cannot affect action.

Interoceptive bias

The tendency for sensory input about the body itself to affect one's judgement about external, unrelated circumstances. (As for example, in parole judges who are more lenient when fed and rested.)

Money illusion

The tendency to concentrate on the nominal value (face value) of money rather than its value in terms of purchasing power.

Moral credential effect

Occurs when someone who does something good gives themselves permission to be less good in the future.

Non-adaptive choice switching

After experiencing a bad outcome with a decision problem, the tendency to avoid the choice previously made when faced with the same decision problem again, even though the choice was optimal. Also known as "once bitten, twice shy" or "hot stove effect".

Omission bias

The tendency to judge harmful actions (commissions) as worse, or less moral, than equally harmful inactions (omissions).

Optimism bias

The tendency to be over-optimistic, underestimating greatly the probability of undesirable outcomes and overestimating favorable and pleasing outcomes (see also wishful thinking, valence effect, positive outcome bias).

Ostrich effect

Ignoring an obvious (negative) situation.

Outcome bias

The tendency to judge a decision by its eventual outcome instead of based on the quality of the decision at the time it was made.

Pessimism bias

The tendency for some people, especially those suffering from depression, to overestimate the likelihood of negative things happening to them.

Present bias

The tendency of people to give stronger weight to payoffs that are closer to the present time when considering trade-offs between two future moments.

Plant blindness

The tendency to ignore plants in their environment and a failure to recognize and appreciate the utility of plants to life on earth.

Probability matching

Sub-optimal matching of the probability of choices with the probability of reward in a stochastic context.

Pro-innovation bias

The tendency to have an excessive optimism towards an invention or innovation's usefulness throughout society, while often failing to identify its limitations and weaknesses.

Projection bias

The tendency to overestimate how much our future selves share one's current preferences, thoughts and values, thus leading to sub-optimal choices.

Proportionality bias

Our innate tendency to assume that big events have big causes, may also explain our tendency to accept conspiracy theories.

Recency illusion

The illusion that a phenomenon one has noticed only recently is itself recent. Often used to refer to linguistic phenomena; the illusion that a word or language usage that one has noticed only recently is an innovation when it is, in fact, long-established (see also frequency illusion).

Systematic bias

Judgement that arises when targets of differentiating judgement become subject to effects of regression that are not equivalent.

Risk compensation / Peltzman effect

The tendency to take greater risks when perceived safety increases.

Surrogation

Losing sight of the strategic construct that a measure is intended to represent, and subsequently acting as though the measure is the construct of interest.

Parkinson's law of triviality

The tendency to give disproportionate weight to trivial issues. Also known as bikeshedding, this bias explains why an organization may avoid specialized or complex subjects, such as the design of a nuclear reactor, and instead focus on something easy to grasp or rewarding to the average participant, such as the design of an adjacent bike shed.

Unconscious bias

Also known as implicit biases, are the underlying attitudes and stereotypes that people unconsciously attribute to another person or group of people that affect how they understand and engage with them. Many researchers suggest that unconscious bias occurs automatically as the brain makes quick judgments based on past experiences and background.

Unit bias

The standard suggested amount of consumption (e.g., food serving size) is perceived to be appropriate, and a person would consume it all even if it is too much for this particular person.

Weber–Fechner law

Difficulty in comparing small differences in large quantities.

Women are wonderful effect

A tendency to associate more positive attributes with women than with men.

Confirmation bias

Backfire effect

The reaction to disconfirming evidence by strengthening one's previous beliefs. Note: the existence of this bias as a widespread phenomenon has been disputed in empirical studies

Confirmation bias

The tendency to search for, interpret, focus on and remember information in a way that confirms one's preconceptions.

Congruence bias

The tendency to test hypotheses exclusively through direct testing, instead of testing possible alternative hypotheses.

Continued influence effect

The tendency to believe previously learned misinformation even after it has been corrected. Misinformation can still influence inferences one generates after a correction has occurred. cf. Backfire effect

Experimenter's orexpectation bias

The tendency for experimenters to believe, certify, and publish data that agree with their expectations for the outcome of an experiment, and to disbelieve, discard, or downgrade the corresponding weightings for data that appear to conflict with those expectations.

Observer-expectancy effect

When a researcher expects a given result and therefore unconsciously manipulates an experiment or misinterprets data in order to find it (see also subject-expectancy effect).

Selective perception

The tendency for expectations to affect perception.

Semmelweis reflex

The tendency to reject new evidence that contradicts a paradigm.

Extension neglect

Base rate fallacy orBase rate neglect

The tendency to ignore general information and focus on information only pertaining to the specific case, even when the general information is more important.

Compassion fade

The predisposition to behave more compassionately towards a small number of identifiable victims than to a large number of anonymous ones.

Conjunction fallacy

The tendency to assume that specific conditions are more probable than a more general version of those same conditions. For example, subjects in one experiment perceived the probability of a woman being both a bank teller and a feminist as more likely than the probability of her being a bank teller.

Duration neglect

The neglect of the duration of an episode in determining its value.

Hyperbolic discounting

Discounting is the tendency for people to have a stronger preference for more immediate payoffs relative to later payoffs. Hyperbolic discounting leads to choices that are inconsistent over time – people make choices today that their future selves would prefer not to have made, despite using the same reasoning. Also known as current moment bias, present-bias, and related to Dynamic inconsistency. A good example of this: a study showed that when making food choices for the coming week, 74% of participants chose fruit, whereas when the food choice was for the current day, 70% chose chocolate.

Insensitivity to sample size

The tendency to under-expect variation in small samples.

Less-is-better effect

The tendency to prefer a smaller set to a larger set judged separately, but not jointly.

Neglect of probability

The tendency to completely disregard probability when making a decision under uncertainty.

Scope neglect orscope insensitivity

The tendency to be insensitive to the size of a problem when evaluating it. For example, being willing to pay as much to save 2,000 children or 20,000 children

Zero-risk bias

Preference for reducing a small risk to zero over a greater reduction in a larger risk.

Truthiness

Belief bias

An effect where someone's evaluation of the logical strength of an argument is biased by the believability of the conclusion.

Illusory truth effect

A tendency to believe that a statement is true if it is easier to process, or if it has been stated multiple times, regardless of its actual veracity. These are specific cases of truthiness.

Rhyme as reason effect

Rhyming statements are perceived as more truthful.

Subjective validation

Perception that something is true if a subject's belief demands it to be true. Also assigns perceived connections between coincidences.

Logical fallacy

Berkson's paradox

The tendency to misinterpret statistical experiments involving conditional probabilities.

Gambler's fallacy

The tendency to think that future probabilities are altered by past events, when in reality they are unchanged. The fallacy arises from an erroneous conceptualization of the law of large numbers. For example, "I've flipped heads with this coin five times consecutively, so the chance of tails coming out on the sixth flip is much greater than heads."

Hot-hand fallacy

The "hot-hand fallacy" (also known as the "hot hand phenomenon" or "hot hand") is the belief that a person who has experienced success with a random event has a greater chance of further success in additional attempts.

Illicit transference

Occurs when a term in the distributive (referring to every member of a class) and collective (referring to the class itself as a whole) sense are treated as equivalent. The two variants of this fallacy are the fallacy of composition and the fallacy of division.

Irrational escalation orEscalation of commitment

The phenomenon where people justify increased investment in a decision, based on the cumulative prior investment, despite new evidence suggesting that the decision was probably wrong. Also known as the sunk cost fallacy.

Plan continuation bias

Failure to recognize that the original plan of action is no longer appropriate for a changing situation or for a situation that is different than anticipated.

Subadditivity effect

The tendency to judge the probability of the whole to be less than the probabilities of the parts.

Time-saving bias

Underestimations of the time that could be saved (or lost) when increasing (or decreasing) from a relatively low speed and overestimations of the time that could be saved (or lost) when increasing (or decreasing) from a relatively high speed.

Zero-sum bias

A bias whereby a situation is incorrectly perceived to be like a zero-sum game (i.e., one person gains at the expense of another).

Apophenia

Clustering illusion

The tendency to overestimate the importance of small runs, streaks, or clusters in large samples of random data (that is, seeing phantom patterns).

Illusory correlation

Inaccurately perceiving a relationship between two unrelated events.

Pareidolia

A vague and random stimulus (often an image or sound) is perceived as significant, e.g., seeing images of animals or faces in clouds, the man in the moon, and hearing non-existent hidden messages on records played in reverse.

Framing effect

Contrast effect

The enhancement or reduction of a certain stimulus' perception when compared with a recently observed, contrasting object.

Decoy effect

Preferences for either option A or B change in favor of option B when option C is presented, which is completely dominated by option B (inferior in all respects) and partially dominated by option A.

Default effect

When given a choice between several options, the tendency to favor the default one.

Denomination effect

The tendency to spend more money when it is denominated in small amounts (e.g., coins) rather than large amounts (e.g., bills).

Distinction bias

The tendency to view two options as more dissimilar when evaluating them simultaneously than when evaluating them separately.

Framing effect

Drawing different conclusions from the same information, depending on how that information is presented.

Egocentric bias

Forer effect orBarnum effect

The observation that individuals will give high accuracy ratings to descriptions of their personality that supposedly are tailored specifically for them, but are in fact vague and general enough to apply to a wide range of people. This effect can provide a partial explanation for the widespread acceptance of some beliefs and practices, such as astrology, fortune telling, graphology, and some types of personality tests.

Illusion of control

The tendency to overestimate one's degree of influence over other external events.

Illusion of validity

Overestimating the accurracy of one's judgments, especially when available information is consistent or inter-correlated.

Overconfidence effect

Excessive confidence in one's own answers to questions. For example, for certain types of questions, answers that people rate as "99% certain" turn out to be wrong 40% of the time.

Planning fallacy

The tendency to underestimate one's own task-completion times.

Restraint bias

The tendency to overestimate one's ability to show restraint in the face of temptation.

Familiarity principle

Mere exposure effect

The tendency to express undue liking for things merely because of familiarity with them.

Cognitive dissonance

Normalcy bias

The refusal to plan for, or react to, a disaster which has never happened before.

Social

Attribution bias

Actor-observer bias

The tendency for explanations of other individuals' behaviors to overemphasize the influence of their personality and underemphasize the influence of their situation (see also Fundamental attribution error), and for explanations of one's own behaviors to do the opposite (that is, to overemphasize the influence of our situation and underemphasize the influence of our own personality).

Extrinsic incentives bias

An exception to the fundamental attribution error, when people view others as having (situational) extrinsic motivations and (dispositional) intrinsic motivations for oneself

Fundamental attribution error

The tendency for people to over-emphasize personality-based explanations for behaviors observed in others while under-emphasizing the role and power of situational influences on the same behavior (see also actor-observer bias, group attribution error, positivity effect, and negativity effect).

Hostile attribution bias

The "hostile attribution bias" is the tendency to interpret others' behaviors as having hostile intent, even when the behavior is ambiguous or benign.

Intentionality bias

Tendency to judge human action to be intentional rather than accidental.

Just-world hypothesis

The tendency for people to want to believe that the world is fundamentally just, causing them to rationalize an otherwise inexplicable injustice as deserved by the victim(s).

Moral luck

The tendency for people to ascribe greater or lesser moral standing based on the outcome of an event.

Puritanical bias

Refers to the tendency to attribute cause of an undesirable outcome or wrongdoing by an individual to a moral deficiency or lack of self-control rather than taking into account the impact of broader societal determinants .

Self-serving bias

The tendency to claim more responsibility for successes than failures. It may also manifest itself as a tendency for people to evaluate ambiguous information in a way beneficial to their interests (see also group-serving bias).

Ultimate attribution error

Similar to the fundamental attribution error, in this error a person is likely to make an internal attribution to an entire group instead of the individuals within the group.

Association fallacy

Authority bias

The tendency to attribute greater accuracy to the opinion of an authority figure (unrelated to its content) and be more influenced by that opinion.

Cheerleader effect

The tendency for people to appear more attractive in a group than in isolation.

Halo effect

The tendency for a person's positive or negative traits to "spill over" from one personality area to another in others' perceptions of them (see also physical attractiveness stereotype).

Conformity bias

Availability cascade

A self-reinforcing process in which a collective belief gains more and more plausibility through its increasing repetition in public discourse (or "repeat something long enough and it will become true").

Bandwagon effect

The tendency to do (or believe) things because many other people do (or believe) the same. Related to groupthink and herd behavior.

Courtesy bias

The tendency to give an opinion that is more socially correct than one's true opinion, so as to avoid offending anyone.

Groupthink

The psychological phenomenon that occurs within a group of people in which the desire for harmony or conformity in the group results in an irrational or dysfunctional decision-making outcome. Group members try to minimize conflict and reach a consensus decision without critical evaluation of alternative viewpoints by actively suppressing dissenting viewpoints, and by isolating themselves from outside influences.

Social desirability bias

The tendency to over-report socially desirable characteristics or behaviours in oneself and under-report socially undesirable characteristics or behaviours. See also: § Courtesy bias.

Cognitive dissonance

Ben Franklin effect

A person who has performed a favor for someone is more likely to do another favor for that person than they would be if they had received a favor from that person.

Egocentric bias

Bias blind spot

The tendency to see oneself as less biased than other people, or to be able to identify more cognitive biases in others than in oneself.

Defensive attribution hypothesis

Attributing more blame to a harm-doer as the outcome becomes more severe or as personal or situational similarity to the victim increases.

Egocentric bias

Occurs when people claim more responsibility for themselves for the results of a joint action than an outside observer would credit them with.

False consensus effect

The tendency for people to overestimate the degree to which others agree with them.

False uniqueness bias

The tendency of people to see their projects and themselves as more singular than they actually are.

Illusion of asymmetric insight

People perceive their knowledge of their peers to surpass their peers' knowledge of them.

Illusion of transparency

The tendency for people to overestimate the degree to which their personal mental state is known by others, and to overestimate how well they understand others' personal mental states.

Illusory superiority

Overestimating one's desirable qualities, and underestimating undesirable qualities, relative to other people. (Also known as "Lake Wobegon effect", "better-than-average effect", or "superiority bias".)

Naïve cynicism

Expecting more egocentric bias in others than in oneself.

Naïve realism

The belief that we see reality as it really is – objectively and without bias; that the facts are plain for all to see; that rational people will agree with us; and that those who don't are either uninformed, lazy, irrational, or biased.

Trait ascription bias

The tendency for people to view themselves as relatively variable in terms of personality, behavior, and mood while viewing others as much more predictable.

Third-person effect

A hypothesized tendency to believe that mass communicated media messages have a greater effect on others than on themselves. As of 2020, the third-person effect has yet to be reliably demonstrated in a scientific context.

Uncategorized

Group attribution error

The biased belief that the characteristics of an individual group member are reflective of the group as a whole or the tendency to assume that group decision outcomes reflect the preferences of group members, even when information is available that clearly suggests otherwise.

Pygmalion effect

The phenomenon whereby others' expectations of a target person affect the target person's performance.

Reactance

The urge to do the opposite of what someone wants you to do out of a need to resist a perceived attempt to constrain your freedom of choice (see also Reverse psychology).

Reactive devaluation

Devaluing proposals only because they purportedly originated with an adversary.

Social comparison bias

The tendency, when making decisions, to favour potential candidates who don't compete with one's own particular strengths.

Shared information bias

Known as the tendency for group members to spend more time and energy discussing information that all members are already familiar with (i.e., shared information), and less time and energy discussing information that only some members are aware of (i.e., unshared information).

Worse-than-average effect

A tendency to believe ourselves to be worse than others at tasks which are difficult.

Ingroup bias

Ingroup bias

The tendency for people to give preferential treatment to others they perceive to be members of their own groups.

Not invented here

Aversion to contact with or use of products, research, standards, or knowledge developed outside a group. Related to IKEA effect.

Outgroup homogeneity bias

Individuals see members of their own group as being relatively more varied than members of other groups.

False priors

Sexual overperception bias / Sexual underperception bias

The tendency to over-/underestimate sexual interest of another person in oneself.

Memory

Uncategorized

Bizarreness effect

Bizarre material is better remembered than common material.

Choice-supportive bias

The tendency to remember one's choices as better than they actually were.

Conservatism or Regressive bias

Tendency to remember high values and high likelihoods/probabilities/frequencies as lower than they actually were and low ones as higher than they actually were. Based on the evidence, memories are not extreme enough.

Consistency bias

Incorrectly remembering one's past attitudes and behaviour as resembling present attitudes and behaviour.

Context effect

That cognition and memory are dependent on context, such that out-of-context memories are more difficult to retrieve than in-context memories (e.g., recall time and accuracy for a work-related memory will be lower at home, and vice versa).

Cross-race effect

The tendency for people of one race to have difficulty identifying members of a race other than their own.

Cryptomnesia

A form of misattribution where a memory is mistaken for imagination, because there is no subjective experience of it being a memory.

Egocentric bias

Recalling the past in a self-serving manner, e.g., remembering one's exam grades as being better than they were, or remembering a caught fish as bigger than it really was.

Fading affect bias

A bias in which the emotion associated with unpleasant memories fades more quickly than the emotion associated with positive events.

False memory

A form of misattribution where imagination is mistaken for a memory.

Generation effect (Self-generation effect)

That self-generated information is remembered best. For instance, people are better able to recall memories of statements that they have generated than similar statements generated by others.

Google effect

The tendency to forget information that can be found readily online by using Internet search engines.

Humor effect

That humorous items are more easily remembered than non-humorous ones, which might be explained by the distinctiveness of humor, the increased cognitive processing time to understand the humor, or the emotional arousal caused by the humor.

Lag effect

The phenomenon whereby learning is greater when studying is spread out over time, as opposed to studying the same amount of time in a single session. See also spacing effect.

Leveling and sharpening

Memory distortions introduced by the loss of details in a recollection over time, often concurrent with sharpening or selective recollection of certain details that take on exaggerated significance in relation to the details or aspects of the experience lost through leveling. Both biases may be reinforced over time, and by repeated recollection or re-telling of a memory.

Levels-of-processing effect

That different methods of encoding information into memory have different levels of effectiveness.

List-length effect

A smaller percentage of items are remembered in a longer list, but as the length of the list increases, the absolute number of items remembered increases as well. For example, consider a list of 30 items ("L30") and a list of 100 items ("L100"). An individual may remember 15 items from L30, or 50%, whereas the individual may remember 40 items from L100, or 40%. Although the percent of L30 items remembered (50%) is greater than the percent of L100 (40%), more L100 items (40) are remembered than L30 items (15).

Misinformation effect

Memory becoming less accurate because of interference from post-event information.

Modality effect

That memory recall is higher for the last items of a list when the list items were received via speech than when they were received through writing.

Mood-congruent memory bias

The improved recall of information congruent with one's current mood.

Negativity bias or Negativity effect

Psychological phenomenon by which humans have a greater recall of unpleasant memories compared with positive memories. (see also actor-observer bias, group attribution error, positivity effect, and negativity effect).

Next-in-line effect

When taking turns speaking in a group using a predetermined order (e.g. going clockwise around a room, taking numbers, etc.) people tend to have diminished recall for the words of the person who spoke immediately before them.

Part-list cueing effect

That being shown some items from a list and later retrieving one item causes it to become harder to retrieve the other items.

Peak-end rule

That people seem to perceive not the sum of an experience but the average of how it was at its peak (e.g., pleasant or unpleasant) and how it ended.

Picture superiority effect

The notion that concepts that are learned by viewing pictures are more easily and frequently recalled than are concepts that are learned by viewing their written word form counterparts.

Positivity effect (Socioemotional selectivity theory)

That older adults favor positive over negative information in their memories.

Serial position effect

That items near the end of a sequence are the easiest to recall, followed by the items at the beginning of a sequence; items in the middle are the least likely to be remembered.

Processing difficulty effect

That information that takes longer to read and is thought about more (processed with more difficulty) is more easily remembered.

Reminiscence bump

The recalling of more personal events from adolescence and early adulthood than personal events from other lifetime periods.

Self-relevance effect

That memories relating to the self are better recalled than similar information relating to others.

Source confusion

Confusing episodic memories with other information, creating distorted memories.

Spacing effect

That information is better recalled if exposure to it is repeated over a long span of time rather than a short one.

Spotlight effect

The tendency to overestimate the amount that other people notice your appearance or behavior.

Stereotypical bias

Memory distorted towards stereotypes (e.g., racial or gender).

Suffix effect

Diminishment of the recency effect because a sound item is appended to the list that the subject is not required to recall.

Suggestibility

A form of misattribution where ideas suggested by a questioner are mistaken for memory.

Tachypsychia

When time perceived by the individual either lengthens, making events appear to slow down, or contracts.

Telescoping effect

The tendency to displace recent events backwards in time and remote events forward in time, so that recent events appear more remote, and remote events, more recent.

Testing effect

The fact that you more easily remember information you have read by rewriting it instead of rereading it.

Tip of the tongue phenomenon

When a subject is able to recall parts of an item, or related information, but is frustratingly unable to recall the whole item. This is thought to be an instance of "blocking" where multiple similar memories are being recalled and interfere with each other.

Travis Syndrome

Overestimating the significance of the present. It is related to chronological snobbery with possibly an appeal to novelty logical fallacy being part of the bias.

Verbatim effect

That the "gist" of what someone has said is better remembered than the verbatim wording. This is because memories are representations, not exact copies.

von Restorff effect

That an item that sticks out is more likely to be remembered than other items.

Zeigarnik effect

That uncompleted or interrupted tasks are remembered better than completed ones.