-
Notifications
You must be signed in to change notification settings - Fork 1
New issue
Have a question about this project? Sign up for a free GitHub account to open an issue and contact its maintainers and the community.
By clicking “Sign up for GitHub”, you agree to our terms of service and privacy statement. We’ll occasionally send you account related emails.
Already on GitHub? Sign in to your account
Spatial frequency is a underrepresented #12
Comments
|
Spatial frequency is a primary driver of contrast, this is peer reviewed, scientific consensus going back to the 1960s, and is academic. I have provided multiple bibliographies. That you are not interested in reading them does not mean they don't exist. |
FALSERead it again. The SIMPLE LEVELS have spatial frequency components that you are fully ignoring. |
The "whole section" on spatial frequency is notwithstanding. Here is is pasted below with discussion:
No, text is easiest to read within the range of 0.2° to 2.0° of visual angle (Legge et alia)
We speak of the spatial frequency of stimuli. The size of the retinal image is defined as visual angle. The capital E is 2.5 cycles vertically.
Spatial frequency is not "good or bad" it is high or low, and the question then is what is the appropriate spatial frequency for best fluent readability for a given acuity.
GENERALLY FALSEIt is true that the CS curve is based on the JND threshold, but it is absolutely false that all of the supra threshold is in contrast constancy, and that is a misreading or misunderstanding of the literature. The contrast constancy effect IS ALSO a product of spatial frequency, and it is also FAR supra threshold. That is, there is a range from JND to the constancy level that is NOT subject to the constancy effect. And that is particularly wider at higher spatial frequencies. This is academic and ample more recent studies define it more completely. A B. And for the record the typical critical size for fluent body text is about 12cpd.
You are conflating different bits of data, including non-relevant aspects of the CS curve. The CS curve is created using sinusoidal gratings. However, text has a "sharp edge" therefore large bold text is a combination of both low and high spatial frequencies (see signal theory and square waves). PEAK contrast sensitivity is approx. 2 to 4 cpd. However, a basic latin letterform can be defined as ~2.5 cycles. (The vertical dimension of capital E is 2.5 cycles). The scientific consensus on "best readability" is text where letterforms subtend between 0.2° and 2.0° of visual angle. (Lovie-Kitchen, Whittacker, Bailey, Legge et alia)
Except that it is obviously defined in CSS with the CSS reference px which is further defined as 1.278 arc minutes of visual angle.
Which is WHY APCA guidelines are based on a REFERENCE FONT.
This is a bizarre statement. Fonts do not "depend on user preference" they are defined in the CSS, therefore the author definitely knows before hand what font(s) they are choosing.
As for device pixels, we don't need to know that, that is up to the manufacturer, but we do have the GUIDE to that relationship, including how close the device is used, and that is the CSS reference px, as stated above. See this chart:
WCAG 2 ignores it, though there is a single breakpoint at 24px, which happens to be about the point where antialiasing effects come into play. APCA guidelines on matching to the reference font are here. |
The proper visual examples can be seen at the corrected fork of this repo |
@Myndex wrote in #7 (comment):
The text was updated successfully, but these errors were encountered: